<ul><li><p>The Handbook of Morphology </p><p>Edited by: Andrew Spencer and ARNOLD ZWICKY (Stanford University and Ohio State </p><p>University) </p><p>Series: Blackwell Handbooks in Linguistics </p><p>List of Contributors. </p><p>List of Abbreviations. </p><p>Introduction (Andrew Spencer and Arnold M. Zwicky </p><p>Part I: The Phenomena. </p><p>1. Inflection (Gregory T. Stump) </p><p>2. Derivation (Robert Beard) </p><p>3. Compounding (Nigel Fabb) </p><p>4. Incorporation (Donna B. Gerdts) </p><p>5. Clitics (Aaron L. Halpern) </p><p>6. Morphophonological Operations (Andrew Spencer) </p><p>7. Phonological Constraints on Morphological Rules (Andrew Carstairs-McCarthy) </p><p>Part II: Morphology and Grammar. </p><p>8. Morphology and Syntax (Hagit Borer) </p><p>9. Morphology and Agreement (Greville G. Corbett) </p><p>10. Morphology and Argument Structure (Louisa Sadler and Andrew Spencer) </p><p>11. Morphology and the Lexicon: Lexicalization and Productivity (Mark Aronoff and Frank </p><p>Anshen) </p><p>12. Morphology and Lexical Semantics (Beth Levin and Malka Rappaport Hovav) </p><p>13. Morphology and Pragmatics (Ferenc Kiefer) </p><p>Part III: Theoretical Issues. </p><p>14. Prosodic Morphology: (John J. McCarthy and Alan S. Prince) </p><p>15. Word Syntax (Jindrich Toman) </p><p>16. Paradigmatic Structure: Inflectional Paradigms and Morphological Classes (Andrew </p><p>Carstairs-McCarthy) </p><p>17. Morphology as Component or Module: Mapping Principle Approaches (Richard Sproat) </p></li><li><p>Part IV: Morphology in a Wider Setting. </p><p>18. Diachronic Morphology (Brian D. Joseph) </p><p>19. Morphology and Language Acquisition (Eve V. Clark) </p><p>20. Morphology and Aphasia (William Badecker and Alfonso Caramazza) </p><p>21. Morphology and Word Recognition (James M. McQueen and Anne Cutler) </p><p>22. Morphology in Language Production with Special Reference to Connectionism (Joseph </p><p>Paul Stemberger) </p><p>Part V: Morphological Sketches of Individual Languages. </p><p>23. Archi (Caucasian - Daghestanian (Aleksandr E. Kibrik) </p><p>24. Celtic (Indo-European) (James Fife and Gareth King) </p><p>25. Chichewa (Bantu) (Sam A. Mchombo) </p><p>26. Chukchee (Paleo-Siberian) (Irina A Muravyova) </p><p>27. Hua (Papuan) (John Haiman) </p><p>28. Malagasy (Austronesian) (Edward L. Keenan and Maria Polinsky) </p><p>29. Qafar (East Cushitic) (Richard J. Hayward) </p><p>30. Slave (Northern Athapaskan) (Keren Rice) </p><p>31. Wari (Amazonian) (Daniel L. Everett) </p><p>32. Warumungu (Australian - Pama - Nyungan) (Jane Simpson) </p><p>References. </p><p>Subject Index. </p><p>Author Index. </p></li><li><p>IntroductionIntroductionIntroductionIntroduction </p><p>ANDREW SPENCER ANDREW SPENCER ANDREW SPENCER ANDREW SPENCER ANDANDANDAND ARNOLD M. ZWICKY ARNOLD M. ZWICKY ARNOLD M. ZWICKY ARNOLD M. ZWICKY </p><p>Morphology is at the conceptual centre of linguistics. This is not because it is the dominant </p><p>subdiscipline, but because morphology is the study of word structure, and words are at the interface </p><p>between phonology, syntax and semantics. Words have phonological properties, they articulate </p><p>together to form phrases and sentences, their form often reflects their syntactic function, and their </p><p>parts are often composed of meaningful smaller pieces. In addition, words contract relationships with </p><p>each other by virtue of their form; that is, they form paradigms and lexical groupings. For this reason, </p><p>morphology is something all linguists have to know about. The centrality of the word brings with it </p><p>two important challenges. First, there is the question of what governs morphological form: how is </p><p>allomorphy to be described? The second is the question of what governs the syntactic and semantic </p><p>function of morphological units, and how these interact with syntax and semantics proper. </p><p>There is a less enviable aspect to this centrality. Morphology has been called the Poland of linguistics </p><p> at the mercy of imperialistically minded neighbours. In the heyday of American structuralism, </p><p>morphology and phonology were the principal objects of study. Monographs entitled The Grammar of </p><p>L, for some language L, would frequently turn out to consist of the phoneme system of L and its </p><p>morphology. However, the study of morphology in generative linguistics was largely eclipsed by </p><p>phonology and syntax in the early days (though it is up to historians of linguistics to say exactly why). </p><p>Ultimately, it came to be that when morphology was considered at all, it was regarded as essentially </p><p>either a part of phonology or a part of syntax. True, there were a number of important works on </p><p>morphology, mainly inflectional morphology, such as Kiefers (1973) work on Swedish, Bierwischs </p><p>(1967) study of German and Warburton's (1973) paper on Greek inflection; but it was not until Halle's </p><p>(1973) short programmatic statement that linguistics at large began to appreciate that there was a </p><p>vacuum in linguistic theory where morphology should be. This was followed in 1974 by two </p><p>particularly influential MIT dissertations, later published as Aronoff (1976) and Siegel (1979), </p><p>proposing radically different approaches to the subject. </p><p>Siegel's theory of Level Ordering brought with it a new way of looking at the phonologymorphology </p><p>interface, which ultimately grew into Kiparsky's (1982a) Lexical Phonology. Siegel argued that those </p><p>affixes in English which never affect stress (and which do not trigger other lexical phonological </p><p>alternations) such as -ness are attached after stress rules have applied. These are the # boundary </p><p>affixes of SPE (Chomsky and Halle 1968), renamed Class II. The + boundary (Class I) affixes are those </p><p>which do affect stress, such as -ity, and they are attached before the stress rules. This led to an </p><p>interesting prediction about the linear order of affixes: Class I affixes appear nearer the root than </p><p>Class II affixes. This generalization is largely true, though it has been regularly pointed out since </p><p>Aronoff (1976) that it is not entirely true. Fabb (1988) has argued that even if it is true, the Level </p><p>Ordering Hypothesis is not sufficient to explain affix ordering in its entirety, and that alternative </p><p>conceptions which do give reasonably broad coverage can also handle the Level Ordering phenomena. </p><p>Lexical Phonology is generally associated with Level Ordering (though a number of lexical </p><p>phonologists have distanced themselves from it; cf. Booij and Rubach 1987). However, the leading </p><p>Theoretical Linguistics Morphology </p><p>10.1111/b.9780631226949.2001.00003.x </p><p>SubjectSubjectSubjectSubject</p><p>DOI:DOI:DOI:DOI:</p><p>Sayfa 1 / 8Introduction : The Handbook of Morphology : Blackwell Reference Online</p><p>28.12.2007http://www.blackwellreference.com/subscriber/uid=532/tocnode?id=g9780631226949...</p></li><li><p>ideas of the model do not actually require Level Ordering. The main thrust of Kiparsky's theory is to </p><p>emphasize the traditional distinction between morphophonemic alternations and automatic </p><p>alternations. The morphophonemic alternations are generally mappings from sets of phonemes into </p><p>sets of phonemes (Structure Preservation), apply in contexts which are not defined in purely </p><p>phonological terms, often have lexical exceptions, can be cancelled by native speakers (e.g. in loan </p><p>phonology), and generally apply only within words. The automatic alternations are generally </p><p>allophonic (non-Structure Preserving), speakers are generally not aware of them, they apply to </p><p>monomorphemic forms, and they often apply across words. Kiparsky argued that morphophonemic </p><p>alternations are actually triggered by morphological operations of affixation. As an affix is added (or a </p><p>cycle of affixation with a level is completed), the battery of lexical phonological rules applies. This </p><p>gives rise to various types of cyclic effect, and accounts for a good many of the properties of the two </p><p>types of rule. </p><p>This innovation was more significant for the development of phonology than for that of morphology, </p><p>except that it (a) began to draw the attention of phonologists to morphology, and (b) tended to </p><p>strengthen the view that morphology was the poor relation to phonology. Lexical Phonology retains </p><p>the assumptions of SPE that every microgram of phonological regularity has to be squeezed out of the </p><p>system before we have to throw in the towel and admit that it's mere allomorphy. As a result, there </p><p>have been very few attempts to examine the extent to which the alternations might themselves have a </p><p>morphological function. To some extent this is addressed in Spencer's chapter, MORPHOPHONOLOGICAL </p><p>OPERATIONS and also in Carstairs-McCarthy's PHONOLOGICAL CONSTRAINTS ON MORPHOLOGICAL RULES. </p><p>While Chomsky's original syntactic theorizing overturned structuralist thinking about that discipline, </p><p>seminal studies in morphology from MIT served to strengthen structuralist assumptions. McCarthy </p><p>(1979) showed that root-and- pattern morphology could be handled very nicely as a kind of affixation </p><p>by adopting the then new theory of Autosegmental Phonology. Lieber (1980) built a theory of the </p><p>lexicon in which affixes are almost exactly like fully-fledged lexical items, with a phonology, a </p><p>meaning, a syntactic category and a subcategorization frame. At the same time, Selkirk (1982) and E. </p><p>Williams (1981b) were arguing that word structure is very much like phrase structure, by applying X-</p><p>bar syntax to words. This very influential approach is reviewed in Toman's chapter, WORD SYNTAX. </p><p>Central to the debate over the relationship between phonology and morphology is a long-standing </p><p>question in structuralist linguistics, whether morphology is best thought of in terms of Item-and-</p><p>Process or Item-and-Arrangement. In an IA approach, a word is made out of a string (or tree) of </p><p>objects; that is, word formation is the concatenation of morphemes, conceived of as mini-lexemes. In </p><p>an IP approach, forms of a word are the outputs of processes applied to a lexeme. This idea has been </p><p>revivified in various ways. Categorial grammar has been co-opted to develop a formal way of </p><p>describing the idea that affixation be viewed as a process (Hoeksema 1985). In a different vein, and </p><p>working from a different tradition, McCarthy and Prince have studied the way in which non-</p><p>concatenative effects are obtained by parsing out various phonologically defined subparts of words </p><p>and stems before applying affixation (or other operations) to them, and this work is summarized in </p><p>their chapter PROSODIC MORPHOLOGY. </p><p>However, the structuralist idea that words are just like phrases, and that the same set of principles </p><p>applies to both domains, is very attractive, especially to non-morphologists, and it is a theme which </p><p>runs through much of the research on the morphologysyntax interface over the past two decades. </p><p>Its most obvious application is in compounding where almost everyone accepts that words have some </p><p>kind of constituent structure. Somewhat more controversial is the view that derivational morphology </p><p>is like phrase syntax, a thesis that is being explored in the domain of argument structure by Hale and </p><p>Keyser (1993). This assumption was challenged by Aronoff (1976), and has more recently been </p><p>attacked by Anderson (1992), for whom all non-compounding morphology is a-morphous. </p><p>Anderson's strong position is, perhaps, extreme (see Carstairs-McCarthy 1992 for a telling critique). </p><p>However, the idea that morphemes are something other than just very short words which happen to </p><p>be bound is particularly influential amongst morphologists. Many theorists view word formation not </p><p>as the concatenation of two things to form a headed syntax-like structure, but as an operation on a </p><p>lexeme. For such theorists, affixation tends to be thought of as just one type of morphophonological </p><p>operation among several, and not a privileged syntactic process of concatenation. Word formation in </p><p>Aronoff (1976) is accomplished by Word Formation Rules (WFRs), and this leads to a radically different </p><p>conception of word structure. For one thing it opens the way to separating the phonological form of </p><p>Sayfa 2 / 8Introduction : The Handbook of Morphology : Blackwell Reference Online</p><p>28.12.2007http://www.blackwellreference.com/subscriber/uid=532/tocnode?id=g9780631226949...</p></li><li><p>an affix from the morphological function or meaning of which it is an exponent. This is the content of </p><p>the Separation Hypothesis (Beard 1988). It is widely assumed in works on inflection, but Beard argues </p><p>it for derivation too, and surveys a number of the arguments in his chapter, DERIVATION. </p><p>The domain where separationism has been most popular is inflection. Following Matthews's (1972) </p><p>detailed critique of the structuralist notion of morpheme in inflection, Anderson (1977b) began a </p><p>programme of research which took inflections to be the result of word formation rules much like </p><p>those proposed by Aronoff (1976) for derivation, but with complex interactions. This work is </p><p>summarized in Stump's chapter, INFLECTION. </p><p>In Principles and Parameters syntax the importance of functional categories, which include inflectional </p><p>categories, was being stressed throughout the 1980s. At the same time, Baker's dissertation (written </p><p>in 1985 and revised as Baker 1988a) developed an extremely influential view of valency alternations </p><p>based on the idea of incorporation, coded as syntactic head-to-head movement. This meant that, for </p><p>example, the causative form of a verb was treated as a syntactic compound of two verbs, one of them </p><p>a causative. This led to the view that inflectional morphology could be handled in the same way, and </p><p>that an inflectional piece, say, a third-person singular subject in the past tense, was syntactically a </p><p>compound consisting of the verb, an Agreement head, Agr, bearing the features [3sg] and a Tense </p><p>head, T, bearing the feature [+Past] (cf. Pollock 1989). Some general problems with this account are </p><p>discussed in Borer's chapter, MORPHOLOGY AND SYNTAX, and a number of morphologists have pointed </p><p>out problems with the full-blown version of the approach, mainly from allo- morphy (Carstairs-</p><p>McCarthy 1992, Joseph and Smirniotopoulos 1993, Spencer 1992). However, more recently, Halle and </p><p>Marantz (1993) have attempted to combine the separationist tradition in inflection with the functional </p><p>head-movement approach, arguing that only in this way can we capture certain alleged homologies </p><p>between morphological structure and syntactic structure. Their model is discussed in Stump's </p><p>contribution. In addition, Rice shows how the complex and arbitrary-looking structure prefix of Slave </p><p>(Athabaskan) none the less reflects syntactic structure to an interesting degree. </p><p>One of the traditional problems in morphology and lexicology has been defining what is meant by </p><p>word. There are various criteria based on form (which tend to be equivocal) and others based on </p><p>behaviour and function (which tend to be even more equivocal). One symptom of this is the existence </p><p>of elements which bear some of the hallmarks of words and also important features of affixes, </p><p>namely, clitics. Ever since Zwicky's (1977) preliminary typology, there has been interest in this </p><p>problem, and for many phonologists and syntacticians, as well as morphologists, it is an urgent </p><p>practical matter, since both phonology and syntax appeal regularly to...</p></li></ul>
The Handbook of Morphology Edited by Andrew Spencer and Arnold Zwicky The Handbook of Japanese Linguistics Edited by Natsuko Tsujimura The Handbook of Linguistics Edited by Mark Aronoff and Janie Rees-Miller The Handbook of Contemporary Syntactic Theory Edited by Mark Baltin and Chris Collins. The Handbook of Morphology Edited by: Andrew Spencer and ARNOLD ZWICKY (Stanford University and Ohio State University) Series: Blackwell Handbooks in Linguistics List of Contributors. List of Abbreviations.
The Handbook Of Morphology Spencer Zwicky Pdf Free
- Morphology is at the conceptual centre of linguistics. This is not because it is the dominant subdiscipline, but because morphology is the study of word structure, and words are at the interface between phonology, syntax and semantics.
- The Handbook of Morphology Andrew Spencer, Arnold M. Zwicky No preview available - 1991. The Handbook of Morphology Andrew Spencer, Arnold M. Zwicky No preview.
- The Handbook of Morphology Edited by: Andrew Spencer and ARNOLD ZWICKY (Stanford University and Ohio State University) Series: Blackwell Handbooks in Linguistics List of.
The Handbook Of Morphology Spencer Zwicky Pdf Pdf
Main → The Handbook of Morphology (Blackwell Handbooks in Linguistics) The Handbook of Morphology (Blackwell Handbooks in Linguistics) Andrew Spencer, Arnold M.